Skip to main content

Long-term effect of uncomplicated Plasmodium berghei ANKA malaria on memory and anxiety-like behaviour in C57BL/6 mice

Abstract

Background

Cerebral malaria, the main complication of Plasmodium falciparum infection in humans, is associated with persistent neurocognitive sequels both in human disease and the murine experimental model. In recent years, cognitive deficits related to uncomplicated (non-cerebral) malaria have also been reported in chronically exposed residents of endemic areas, but not in some murine experimental models of non-cerebral malaria. This study aimed at evaluating the influence of uncomplicated malaria on different behavioural paradigms associated with memory and anxiety-like parameters in a murine model that has the ability to develop cerebral malaria.

Methods

Plasmodium berghei ANKA-infected and non-infected C57BL/6 mice were used. Development of cerebral malaria was prevented by chloroquine treatment starting on the fourth day of infection. The control group (non-infected mice) were treated with PBS. The effect of uncomplicated malaria infection on locomotor habituation, short and long-term memory and anxious-like behaviour was evaluated 64 days after parasite clearance in assays including open field, object recognition, Y-maze and light/dark tasks.

Results

Plasmodium berghei ANKA-infected mice showed significant long-lasting disturbances reflected by a long-term memory-related behaviour on open field and object recognition tasks, accompanied by an anxious-like phenotype availed on open field and light-dark tasks.

Conclusions

Long-term neurocognitive sequels may follow an uncomplicated malaria episode in an experimental model prone to develop cerebral malaria, even if the infection is treated before the appearance of clinical signs of cerebral impairment.

Background

Malaria, one of the major public health problems in the world, usually manifests with the classic symptoms and signs of fever, sweating, chills and intense headache. It may also result in immunopathological phenomena that accompany the severe forms of the disease [1, 2]. In 1–2% of the cases of infection with Plasmodium falciparum, which account for around 90% of the world cases, cerebral malaria (CM), the most severe complication of falciparum infection, develops, resulting in about 80% of all the malaria deaths, mainly of children, pregnant women and non-immune adults [3]. Survivors may present neurocognitive sequels, such as behavioural changes, learning deficits and other cognitive impairments, in the short or long-term after the clinical episodes [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Neurocognitive impairment has also been consistently recorded in murine models of CM [12, 13].

In recent years, poor performance in cognitive events, mainly related to learning and memory, has also been observed in individuals with uncomplicated malaria episodes in different endemic regions of the world [14,15,16,17,18]. They were not, however, observed in some murine experimental models in which CM is not expected to occur [12, 19]. The appearance of CM in some models seems, therefore, to depend on the host-parasite interaction specific to each situation, involving the genetic background of the host and the parasite species and strains and the nature and intensity of the consequent inflammatory response, determining or not damage to brain tissues and vasculature [20].

The late impact of the uncomplicated malaria on cognitive performance is still poorly understood. The present study aimed at searching for a long-lasting influence of uncomplicated experimental murine malaria on different behavioural paradigms, specifically designed to investigate the cognitive performance and anxiety-like phenotype in mice. Considering that the great majority of cases of human malaria due to P. falciparum present as non-complicated malaria, we decided to use the classical murine experimental model of CM (P. berghei ANKA-infected C57BL/6) treating the mice early (at day 4) in the course of infection to avoid CM development. Our understanding is that this prototype simulates the situation of uncomplicated falciparum malaria, which has the potential of becoming severe and evolving to CM, but will not do so if treated opportunely.

Methods

Animals

Seven-week-old C57BL/6 female, mice weighing 20–25 g, were provided by the “Instituto de Ciência e Tecnologia em BioModelos” of the “Fundação Oswaldo Cruz” (ICTB- Fiocruz, Brazil). Mice were kept in cages containing five animals, housed in racks with an air filtration system in a room kept at 25 °C and 12 h light/dark cycles with free access to food and water.

Infection and treatment of experimental groups

Passage C57BL/6 mice were infected by intraperitoneal (ip) injection with 150 μl of cryopreserved and thawed Plasmodium berghei ANKA expressing green fluorescent protein (GFP) (PbA) [21] parasitized red blood cells. Five days after PbA-inoculation, the whole blood of the passage mice was collected, adjusted to 106 parasitized erythrocytes per 100 μl inoculum, and injected by the ip route in C57BL/6 mice of the experimental groups. In this model of infection, the evolution and establishment of CM occurs between the fifth and sixth day [22]. Parasitaemia was determined by flow cytometry, based on the percentage of GFP+ erythrocytes. Infected mice were treated on the fourth day of infection, with a mean parasitaemia of 2.5% (range 1.1–4.3) before any clinical sign of CM was apparent. The treatment regimen used [chloroquine (CQ) 25 mg/kg (total dose)] was standardized accordingly to the literature for the treatment of human malaria caused by Plasmodium parasites sensitive to this drug [3, 23]. The only difference is that the total dose in mice is given scheduled in 7 days, not 3 (as in humans), since this is the protocol previously show to completely abolish P. berghei parasitaemia in mice [12]. PbA infected/treated C57BL/6 mice will be hereafter referred to as infected mice. Phosphate buffered solution (PBS) was administered by gavage to the control (non-infected mice) group that, otherwise, underwent all manipulation procedures to which the experimental group (infected mice) were subjected. Eleven animals were used per group (of infected or non-infected mice) in two rounds of experiments, totalling 22 animals per group.

An experiment aimed at excluding the existence of a possible effect of chloroquine on learning and anxious behaviour was carried out in a group of non-infected mice before the experiments that used treatment in infected mice. Two groups of (twelve animals each) chloroquine treated or non-treated (PBS treated, control) mice were evaluated for behaviour tests and anxiety phenotype, in two rounds of experiments, totalling 24 animals per group.

Evaluation of behavioural tasks

Sixty-four days after treatment (Fig. 1), animals were evaluated on open field, object recognition, Y-maze and light/dark tasks. Before the sessions, mice were acclimatized in the experimental room for 2 h. All experiments were completed between 2–6 pm and the behavioural performance of mice was analysed using the AnyMaze® software (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL, USA).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experiment flowchart. Plasmodium berghei ANKA-infected C57BL/6 mice received chloroquine treatment for 7 days, starting on the day 4 of infection. Mice were assessed for performance regarding the indicated behavioural tasks on memory and anxiety-like evaluation 64 days after treatment (resting time)

Behavioural tasks

Both open field and object recognition tasks were evaluated in a grey acrylic box (50 × 50 × 50 cm) with a 200 lux intensity white light. Mice were individually placed and then recorded for 10 min with a video-camera (positioned above and at c.90° to the square arena) that was connected to a monitor for further analysis. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% alcohol and dried after all tasks.

Open Field task (OFT)

The OFT was used as previously described [24, 25] and was divided into two daily sessions (i.e. 24 h apart), a training (OFT1) and a test session (OFT2), in order to evaluate locomotor activity (total distance travelled in the OFT training and test sessions); habituation to novelty [(i) a significant decrease in the distance travelled during the first to the third minute on the first training session (OFT1) was used to measure short-term habituation to novelty; (ii) a significant decrease in the total distance travelled in the second test session (OFT2), compared to OFT1 was considered a long-term habituation to novelty]; and anxiety-like phenotypes, evaluated by the decrease in time and distance travelled in the centre of the open field arena in the first OFT exposition.

Novel Object Recognition task (NORT)

The NORT [26] was used, with minor modifications, to evaluate recognition memory. NORT is based on the tendency of mice to discriminate a novel object (NO) from a familiar object (FO). Two OFT sessions were performed before initiating the NORT protocol in order to reduce the novelty of the arena and decrease the anxiety levels. 24 h after the OFT sessions, the mice were submitted to the NORT training session. Animals were individually placed in the periphery of the apparatus with two identical objects and allowed to explore the arena for 10 min. Object exploration was recorded by an experienced observer only when the animal's nose or mouth was in contact with the objects. During training session both objects were novel and the time spent on both objects should be similar. In the NORT test session, 24 h after the training session, the animals were individually placed back in the arena with one FO (the same as in the previous phase) and one NO in order to measure the long-term memory. The time spent exploring the objects was again recorded for 10 min. The results were expressed as percentage of time exploring each object during the test session. The test session should detect a preference for the NO. Mice that recognized the NO as such and explored it for more than 50% of the total time were considered to display successful recognition.

Y-maze task

The Y-maze apparatus consisted of grey wooden walls with 3 identical arms (30 × 8 × 15 cm each at an angle of 120° from the others). The working and short-term memories were evaluated separately using two different protocols and two cohorts of animals. Firstly, the Y-maze task was conducted as described [27] to assess the spontaneous alternation. Mice were placed at the end of one arm and were allowed to explore the maze freely for 8 min without training. An alternation was defined as a complete cycle of consecutive entrances into each of the 3 arms. Percent alternation (PA) was calculated as follows: PA = number of alternations/ (total number of entrances into each arm – 2) × 100. The second approach was a two-trial task with a training phase and a test phase trials [28], separated by an inter trial of 30 min. In the training phase trial, each mouse was individually placed in the maze with one of the 3 arms closed, were allowed freely to explore the 2 arms for 5 min, and then allocated back to the home cage. After 30 min, the animal was again placed in the maze with all 3 arms opened, and was allowed freely to explore all arms. The previously closed arm, opened in the test phase trial, was defined as the new arm. The animal performance was video-recorded for analysis. The time spent and the total distance travelled in the new arm were analysed using AnyMaze® software.

Light/Dark task

The light/dark task was performed as previously described [29]. The apparatus consisted of a rectangular acrylic box with two separate chambers of similar dimensions (50 × 50 × 50 cm). One chamber had black walls and floor and was not illuminated. The other side had white walls and floor and was illuminated by a 100W white lamp that was placed overhead. The two compartments were separated by a wall, which had a small opening at the floor level. Each mouse was placed in the light compartment facing away from the opening and allowed to explore the box for 5 min. The following behavioural parameters were analysed by ANY-Maze software: the number of transitions between compartments and the time spent in the light compartment.

Statistical analysis

The difference in behavioural tasks performance was analysed by a two-way ANOVA test followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test to evaluate the effect of different treatments on locomotor activity and short- and long-term memory. Additionally, unpaired Student’s t-test was used to evaluate differences in the anxiety-related behaviours. However, when the sample standard deviation (SD) was different between the groups, the unpaired t-test with Welch’s correction was used; P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

To address whether uncomplicated P. berghei ANKA (PbA) malaria might influence different behavioural paradigms two months after infection and treatment, C57BL/6 mice were inoculated with 106 parasitized erythrocytes and treated with CQ for seven days from day 4 of infection (Fig. 1).

Effect of CQ treatment on parasitaemia and on recognition memory or anxiety in mice

None of the animals treated with CQ for 7 days showed recrudescence of parasitaemia for up to 4 months of follow-up (Fig. 2) and this therapeutic scheme alone did not influence either the recognition memory (ANOVA: F(1, 52) = 1.390, P = 0.759, Fig. 3a, b) or the anxiety-like parameters (t-test: t(26) = 0.1517, P = 0.8806, Fig. 3c, d), the two important and central behavioural features explored in detail in this study.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effect of chloroquine (CQ) administration for full treatment of PbA infection in C57BL/6 mice. CQ was given by gavage at a concentration of 25 mg/kg daily for 7 days (from day 4 to day 10 of infection, n = 10, combining 2 experiments). This therapeutic scheme does not allow recrudescence of parasitaemia for up to 4 months of follow-up

Fig. 3
figure 3

Influence of chloroquine (CQ) administration on recognition memory and anxiety-like parameters in non-infected animals. CQ was used by gavage at a concentration of 25 mg/kg daily for 7 days (from day 4 to day 10 of infection, n = 10, combining 2 experiments). This therapeutic scheme does not affect the recognition memory (training a; test b) and the anxiety-like parameters (c and d; n = 24, combining 2 experiments) performed 64 days after the administration of CQ on the NO Recognition and Light/Dark task. Control: PBS treated mice. Abbreviations: FO, familiar object; NO, novel object

Effect of uncomplicated malaria on locomotor activity

PbA infected/treated mice showed no difference in the total distance travelled in both OFTs paradigms in relation to the control group (both groups; ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 52.36, P < 0.0001, Fig. 4a).

Fig. 4
figure 4

PbA-infected mice show long-lasting impairment in memory-related task performance. Effect of a treated PbA 4 day-infection on the total distance travelled in open field training and test sessions (a) and on the Δ between the first and the third minute distance travelled (b); effect of PbA 4 day-infection on the time spent in each object in the training (c) and test (d) sessions on the novel object recognition task; and effect of treated PbA 4 day-infection on time spent in each arm in the training (e) and test (f) sessions in the Y-Maze task. Data were analysed by two-way ANOVA, followed by Tukey’s multiple range post-hoc test. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 and ***P < 0.001, comparing the training (OFT1) and test (OFT2) sessions; the familiar (FO) and the novel (NO) object recognition task; and the different arms in the Y-maze task (n = 22 animals/group, combining 2 experiments). See Methods for behavioural task description. Arm 1 (closed and open on training and test sessions, respectively), arms 2 and 3 (open). Abbreviations: Non-inf mice, uninfected and PBS treated mice; PbA-inf mice, infected and chloroquine treated mice

Effect of uncomplicated malaria on different memory-related behaviours

Both infected and non-infected groups significantly decreased the total distance travelled across the OFT test sessions, an important index of long-term habituation memory (ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 52.36, P < 0.0001, Fig. 4a). However, the PbA-infected mice presented no change in the distance travelled between the first and third minute of OFT training and test session (ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 0.2301, P = 0.5365), as compared to non-infected mice in which distance travelled decreased in OFT2 (ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 4.88, P = 0.0392, Fig. 4b), suggesting a short-term habituation to novelty impairment.

No significant difference in the time spent on each familiar object was observed in the Novel Object Recognition task (NORT) training session between non-infected and infected groups (ANOVA: F(1, 77) = 1.088, P = 0.3002; F(1, 80) = 0.01834, P = 0.8926, Fig. 4c). In the test session, however, only non-infected mice recognized the NO (long-term recognition memory) since their NO exploration time was markedly higher than the FO exploration time (ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 35.51, P = 0.0009, Fig. 4d). PbA-infected mice showed no difference between NO and FO exploration time (ANOVA: F(1, 80) = 0.431, P = 0.5281, Fig. 4d).

Moreover, a significant increase was observed in time spent in the arm at which infected mice were initially placed (arm 2) in the training session of the Y-maze task (ANOVA: F(1, 32) = 10.44, P = 0.0024, Fig. 4e) and a different temporal pattern of exploration during testing was registered, comparing the new arm (arm 1) with the other two arms (arms 2 and 3) (ANOVA: F(2, 48) = 4.899, P = 0.0136 and F(2, 48) = 6.283, P = 0.0007 in control mice and F(2, 48) = 2.196, P = 0.1260 and F(2, 48) = 4.899, P = 0.0194 in the PbA infected mice; Fig. 4f). No difference in the percentage of spontaneous arm alternance was observed between PbA-infected and non-infected mice in spatial working memory in the Y-maze task (data not shown).

Effect of uncomplicated malaria on anxiety

A significant decrease in the distance travelled in the centre zone during OFT1 (t-test: t(40) = 2.705, P = 0.0284, Fig. 5b) and a significant reduction in time spent in the light zone in the light/dark task (t-test: t(16) = 2.089, P = 0.0415, Fig. 5c) was observed in PbA infected mice. No difference in time in the centre zone in OFT1 (t-test: t(40) = 0.9658, P = 0.3580) and number of transitions in light/dark task (t-test: t(16) = 1.423, P = 0.1149, Fig. 5a, d) was observed in these animals.

Fig. 5
figure 5

PbA-infected mice presented long-lasting increase in anxious-related behaviour. The distance travelled (a) in the centre zone was evaluated in the open field task training session (n = 22, combining 2 experiments). The time spent in the light compartment (b) was evaluated in the light/dark task (n = 12). Data are reported as the mean ± SEM and were analysed by unpaired Student’s t-test. *P < 0.05 compared to the non-infected animals. See Methods for behavioural task description. Abbreviations: Non-inf mice, uninfected and PBS treated mice; PbA-inf mice, infected and chloroquine treated mice

Discussion

The results described here are, to our knowledge, the first to present robust evidence of a long-term cognitive impairment in murine experimental uncomplicated malaria. PbA-infected C57BL/6 mice exhibited impairment of the recognition memory as well as an increase in the anxiety-like phenotype 64 days after parasite clearance and 71 days after a four days long-infection.

The novel object recognition task (NORT) is an excellent tool to measure parameters related to recognition memory in rodents. Data obtained with this test indicate that the long-term memory of PbA infected mice was impaired even when tested 64 days after treatment. In agreement, deficits in long-term memory after experimental CM have been described both during infection [30,31,32] and 30–40 days after treatment [12, 13]. However, cognitive deficits were not observed in various murine experimental models of non-CM such as PbA-infected BALB/c mice, P. chabaudi chabaudi-infected and P. chabaudi adami-infected C57BL/6 and 17NL Plasmodium yoelii-infected Swiss mice [12, 19]. It must be said that the host-parasite interplay can modulate the pathogenesis of the experimental malaria determining the disease outcome. For example, the profile of immune response to PbA differs according to the mice genetic background, being more inflammatory (Th1 profile) in C57BL/6 mice, than in BALB/c mice (Th2 profile) infected with the same parasite, thus resulting in susceptibility (C57BL/6) or resistance (BALB/c), to the development of CM and, possibly, the emergence or not of neurocognitive dysfunctions [33].

Important features related to cognitive performance, such as short- and long-term habituation to novelty (a parameter strictly related with spatial memory), have been measured in the OFT. Usually, when mice assess for the first time the open field arena, a high rate of locomotor activity is observed. However, over time (normally up to 3 min) this exploratory behaviour decreases significantly, since the stress related to novelty disappears (short-term habituation) [25, 34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42]. Similarly, when mice re-assess the familiar open field arena, they normally show a slower exploratory behaviour called long-term habituation [23]. The results of OFT displayed here may suggest a short-term habituation to novelty impairment in PbA-infected mice. We did not observe any disturbance in total distance travelled comparing PbA-infected mice with non-infected mice in the OFT.

The differential effects of long-term memory for either object recognition (Fig. 4d) or spatial habituation (Fig. 4a) are not surprising, since previous studies have shown that long-term habituation is more robust than other memories, such as that for inhibitory avoidance task [43]. Memory habituation is impaired in surviving CM mice 40 days after treatment [12]. However, a similar study, conducted in a model of uncomplicated experimental malaria (Plasmodium chabaudi adami-infected C57BL/6 mice) [19], evaluated the locomotor performance 15 days after parasite clearance and observed no difference in total distance covered or velocity between infected and control mice. Moreover, CM (PbA-infected C57BL/6) surviving mice did not present any change in locomotor behaviour, although they showed a significant motor deficit 40 days after CQ treatment [13]. These data suggest that the motor impairment is strictly related to severe malaria.

In spite of the recorded impairment in the short-term habituation in OFT1, no long-lasting effect of uncomplicated PbA infection was observed on the Y-maze task, including the specific parameter used to analyze the working memory (percentage of spontaneous arm alternance). On the other hand, the study provides evidence that indicates that PbA-infected mice present long-lasting anxiety-like behaviour, since it was observed a significant decrease in the distance travelled in the centre zone during OFT1, reduced time in the light zone in the light/dark task, and increased time in the arm 2 where they were initially placed in the Y-maze, including a different OFT1 temporal pattern of exploration, mainly in the first minutes of the task. Actually, anxious animals prefer not to be exposed to situations in which they may be exposed to predators, such as an open, novel or light environment. Taken together, these results seem to indicate that the initial evidence concerning the impairment in short-term habituation to novelty may be not due to short-term cognitive deficits but rather to an increase in the anxious-like behaviour.

Increase in anxiety-like behaviour was previously reported in an uncomplicated malaria experimental model (Plasmodium chabaudi adami-infected C57BL/6 mice), 15 days after parasite clearance [19]. Anxious behaviour was also detected, at the high cross-maze [31] and the open field behaviour [32] tasks, at day 5 of infection in the same model of the present study (PbA-infected C57BL/6 mice). However, one must keep in mind that on day 5 when the behaviour was assessed, animals are usually sick (feverish and toxemic) and progressing to or already presenting CM.

Considering the results reported here, we can hypothesize that even a short-term PbA-infection, treated before the appearance of any clinical sign of neurological impairment in C57BL/6 mice [44], a model susceptible to experimental CM, may result in long-term memory deficits and anxiety-like behaviour. Studying this same model, another author has shown that as early as at the fourth day of infection, the leukocyte adhesion process in the cerebral microvasculature had already started, even in the absence of petechial haemorrhage signs and with minimal formation of oedema [22]. This precedes the evident and typical neuropathological CM signs starting on day 5 of infection, with the usual full establishment on day 6 of infection in the model studied and could explain, at least partially, the results reported here. However, leukocyte accumulation has been also shown to occur in the non-CM model of P. berghei NK65 infection of C57BL/6 mice [45], and, moreover, preliminary data shows that no blood-brain barrier breakdown is observed on day 4 in the PbA-C57BL/6 model studied here (unpublished observations).

In this study, the behavioural effects of malaria in C57BL/6 PbA-infected mice were recorded late (70 days after the infection) in animals treated, at day 4, before any clinical manifestation suggestive of brain tissue damage. This late observation of long-term cognitive deficits as sequels of uncomplicated malaria in a CM-prone host-parasite model may be considered as an informative example of the potential situation in human falciparum malaria.

As far as pathogenesis of the observed behavioural effects is concerned, some mechanisms have been proposed to explain behavioural changes in the absence of neural injury, including hypoxia, depression, attention deficit, anxiety, malnutrition, medications and normal aging [46]. We can, therefore, ponder that in the early phases of the infectious process establishment, when minimal changes in the brain physiology and structure may be starting (as the slight leukocyte adhesion and minimum oedema in the cerebral microvasculature, in the absence of petechial haemorrhage recorded at day 4 by Potter et al. [22]), accompanied by the emerging inflammatory environment and probably disturbances in the glial cells homeostasis (directly) and the triggering of an anxious behavioural (indirectly) could influence the cognitive performance of the experimental mice.

Finally, we can speculate that the equivalent time in humans of manifestation of the memory impairment and anxiety-like phenotypes, observed here in mice, based on longevity parameters [47] suggests that cognitive impairments could be detected 7 years after an episode of mild malaria in a human.

Conclusions

Long-lasting cognitive impairments, related to learning and memory, have never been reported as sequels of mild malaria in murine models. Here, evidence of short- and long-term memory deficits 64 days after parasite clearance by CQ treatment of PbA-infected C57BL/6 mice is presented for the first time. Further research in other strains of mice susceptible or insusceptible to develop CM is needed to confirm these data and to elucidate the dynamics of cognitive deficits and anxious related parameters in uncomplicated malaria.

Abbreviations

CM:

cerebral malaria

CQ:

chloroquine

PbA:

Plasmodium berghei ANKA

OFT:

Open Field task

NORT:

Novel Object Recognition task

GFP:

green fluorescent protein

WHO:

World Health Organization

References

  1. Frevert U, Nascer A, Cabrera M, Movila A, Lebert M. Imaging Plasmodium immunobiology in the liver, brain, and lung. Parasitol Int. 2014;63:171–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Silva JR, Ramos Ade S, Machado M, de Moura DF, Neto Z, Canto-Cavalheiro MM, et al. A review of antimalarial plants used in traditional medicine in communities in Portuguese-speaking countries: Brazil, Mozambique, Cape Verde, Guinea-Bissau, São Tomé and Príncipe and Angola. Mem Inst Oswaldo Cruz. 2011;106:142–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. World Health Organization. World Malaria Report. Geneva: WHO; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Mung’Ala-Odera V, Snow RW, Newton CR. The burden of the neurocognitive impairment associated with Plasmodium falciparum malaria in sub-Saharan Africa. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2004;71:64–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Carter JA, Mung’ala-Odera V, Neville BG, Murira G, Mturi N, Musumba C, et al. Persistent neurocognitive impairments associated with severe falciparum malaria in Kenyan children. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat. 2005;76:476–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Carter JA, Lee JA, Gona JK, Murira G, Rimba K, Neville BG, et al. Severe falciparum malaria and acquired childhood language disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:51–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, et al. Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: a prospective study. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e360–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. John CC, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, et al. Cerebral malaria in children is associated with long-term cognitive impairment. Pediatrics. 2008;122:e92–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Giordani B, Opoka RO, John CC, Boivin MJ. Reliability of the Luganda version of the Child Behaviour Checklist in measuring behavioural problems after cerebral malaria. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3:38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Bangirana P, Allebeck P, Boivin MJ, John CC, Page C, Ehnvall A, et al. Cognition, behaviour and academic skills after cognitive rehabilitation in Uganda children surviving severe malaria: a randomised trial. BMC Neurol. 2011;11:96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Overman AA. Computer-based training and cognitive improvement after adult-onset malaria: a case report. Neuro Rehab. 2012;30:101–7.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Reis PA, Comim CM, Hermani F, Silva B, Barichello T, Portella AC, et al. Cognitive dysfunction is sustained after rescue therapy in experimental cerebral malaria, and is reduced by additive antioxidant therapy. PloS Pathog. 2010;6:e1000963.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Dai M, Reznik SE, Spray DC, Weiss LM, Tanowitz HB, Gulinello M, et al. Persistent cognitive and motor deficits after successful antimalarial treatment in murine cerebral malaria. Microbes Infect. 2010;12:1198–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Vitor-Silva S, Reyes-Lecca RC, Pinheiro TR, Lacerda MV. Malaria is associated with poor school performance in endemic area of the Brazilian Amazon. Malar J. 2009;8:230.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Fernando SD, Gunawardena DM, Bandara MR, De Silva D, Carter R, Mendis KN, et al. The impact of repeated malaria attacks on the school performance of children. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2003;69:582–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Thuilliez J, Sissoko MS, Toure OB, Kamate P, Berthelemy JC, Doumbo OK. Malaria and primary education in Mali: a longitudinal study in the village of Donéguébougou. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:324–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Fink G, Olgiati A, Hawela M, Miller JM, Matafwali B. Association between early childhood exposure to malaria and children’s pre-school development evidence from Zambia early childhood development project. Malar J. 2013;12:12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Vorasan N, Pan-Ngum W, Jittamala P, Maneeboonyang W, Rukmanee P, Lawpoolsri S. Long-term impact of the childhood malaria infection on school performance among school children in a malaria endemic area along the Thai-Myanmar border. Malar J. 2015;14:401.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Guha SK, Tillu R, Sood A, Patgaonkar M, Nanavaty IN, Sengupta A, et al. Single episode of mild murine malaria induces neurinflammation, alters microglial profile, impairs adult neurogenesis, and causes deficits in social and anxiety-like behavior. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;42:123–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Li C, Seixas E, Langhorne J. Rodent malaria: the mouse as a model for understanding immune responses and pathology induced by the erythrocytic stages of the parasite. Med Microbiol Immunol. 2001;189:115–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Franke-Fayard B, Trueman H, Ramesar J, Mendoza J, Van der Keur M, Van der Linden R, et al. A Plasmodium berghei reference line that constitutively expresses GFP at a high level throughout the complete life cycle. Mol Biochem Parasitol. 2004;137:23–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Potter S, Chan-Ling T, Ball HJ, Mansour H, Mitchell A, Maluish L, et al. Perforin mediated apoptosis of cerebral microvascular endotelial cells during experimental cerebral malaria. Int J Parasitol. 2006;36:485–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ministério da Saúde. Secretaria de Vigilância em Saúde. Departamento de Vigilância Epidemiológica. Guia prático de tratamento da malária no Brasil/ Ministério da Saúde, Secretaria de Vigilância em Saúde, Departamento de Vigilância Epidemiológica. Brasília: Ministério da Saúde; 2013.

  24. Martins CY, Wernec LG, Carvaho JL, Silva PTB, Andrade GB, Souza MT, et al. Algorithms to predict cerebral malaria in murine models using the SHIPA protocol. Malar J. 2010;9:85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Almeida RF, Ganzella M, Machado DG, Loureiro SO, Leffa D, Quiconzes-Santos A, et al. Olfactory bulbectomy in mice triggers transient and long-lasting behavioural impairments and biochemical hippocampal disturbances. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2017;76:1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Figueiredo CP, Clarke JR, Ledo JH, Ribeiro FC, Costa CV, Melo HM, et al. Memantine rescues transient cognitive impairment caused by high-molecular-weight aβ oligomers but not the persistent impairment induced by low-molecular-weight oligomers. J Neurosci. 2013;33:9626–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Farzanehfar V, Naderi N, Kobarfard F, Faizi M. Determination of dibutyl phthalate neurobehavioral toxicity in mice. Food Chem Toxicol. 2016;94:221–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Jaehne EJ, Corrigan F, Toben C, Jawahar MC, Baune BT. The effect of the antipsychotic drug quetiapine and its metabolite norquetiapine on acute inflammation, memory and anhedonia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2015;135:136–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Almeida RF, Comasseto DD, Ramos DB, Hansel G, Zimmer ER, Loureiro SO, et al. Guanosine anxiolytic-like effect involves adenosinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmitter systems. Mol Neurobiol. 2016;54:423–36.

  30. Desruisseaux MS, Gulinello M, Smith DN, Lee SC, Tsuji M, Weiss LM, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in mice infected with Plasmodium berghei strain ANKA. J Infect Dis. 2008;197:1621–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. de Miranda AS, Brant F, Campos AC, Vieira LB, Rocha NP, Cisalpino D, et al. Evidence for the contribution of adult neurogenesis and hippocampal cell death in experimental cerebral malaria cognitive outcome. Neuroscience. 2015;284:920–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Campos AC, Brant F, Miranda AS, Machado FS, Teixeira AL. Cannabidiol increases survival and promotes rescue of cognitive function in a murine model of cerebral malaria. Neuroscience. 2015;289:166–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. de Kossodo S, Grau GE. Profiles of cytokine production in relation with susceptibility to cerebral malaria. J Immunol. 1993;151:4811–20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Eisenstein SA, Clapper JR, Holmes PV, Piomelli D, Hohmann AG. A role for 2-arachidonoylglycerol and endocannabinoid signaling in the locomotor response to novelty induced by olfactory bulbectomy. Pharm Res. 2010;61:419–29.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Faizi M, Bader PL, Saw N, Nguyen TV, Beraki S, Wyss-Coray T, et al. Thy1-hAPP(Lond/Swe+) mouse model of Alzheimer's disease displays broad behavioral deficits in sensorimotor, cognitive and social function. Brain Behav. 2012;2:142–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Frederick AL, Saborido TP, Stanwood GD. Neurobehavioral phenotyping of G(alphaq) knockout mice reveals impairments in motor functions and spatial working memory without changes in anxiety or behavioral despair. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012;6:29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Galeano P, Blanco CE, Madureira OD, Cuenya L, Kamenetzky GV, Mustaca AE, et al. Long-lasting effects of perinatal asphyxia on exploration, memory and incentive downshift. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2011;29:609–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kalueff AV, Jensen CL, Murphy DL. Locomotory patterns, spatiotemporal organization of exploration and spatial memory in serotonin transporter knockout mice. Brain Res. 2007;1169:87–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Sanderson DJ, Bannerman DM. Competitive short-term and long-term memory processes in spatial habituation. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2011;37:189–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Sare RM, Levine M, Smith CB. Behavioral phenotype of Fmr1 knock-out mice during active phase in an altered light/dark cycle. J Neurosci. 2016;3(2):0035-16.2016.

  41. Sugaya Y, Cagniard B, Yamazaki M, Sakimura K, Kano M. The endocannabinoid 2-arachidonoylglycerol negatively regulates habituation by suppressing excitatory recurrent network activity and reducing long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3588–601.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Zueger M, Urani A, Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Roche M, Harkin A, et al. Olfactory bulbectomy in mice induces alterations in exploratory behavior. Neurosci Lett. 2005;374:142–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Izquierdo LA, Barros DM, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Stress hormones enhance retrieval of fear conditioning acquired either one day or many months before. Behav Pharmacol. 2002;13:203–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Martins CY, Carvalho MJL, Daniel-Ribeiro CT. Challenges in determination of early predictors of cerebral malaria: lessons from the human disease and the experimental models. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2009;16:134–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Shaw TN, Stewart-Hutchinson PJ, Strangward P, Dandamudi DB, Coles JA, Villegas-Mendez A, et al. Perivascular arrest of CD8+ T cells is a signature of experimental cerebral malaria. PloS Pathog. 2015;11:e1005210.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:692–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Dutta S, Sengupta P. Men and mice: relating their ages. Life Sci. 2016;152:244–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

LPS is grateful to the Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Parasitária do Instituto Oswaldo Cruz. The authors thank Ms Aline Moreira for training LPS in P. berghei infection of mice. The Laboratório de Pesquisa em Malária and the Departamento de Bioquímica, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul are INCT (National Institutes of Science & Technology) Associated Laboratories.

Funding

This work is part of LPS’s PhD research supported by a CAPES fellowship. LJMC, DOS and CTDR are supported by CNPq, Brazil, through a Productivity Research Fellowship and LJMC and CTDR are “Cientistas do Nosso Estado” recognised by the Faperj, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LPS was responsible for the realization of all experiments (including infection and treatment of mice; and conduction, observation and data collection/systematisation of cognitive tests), helped in the analysis and interpretation of tests and drafted and reviewed the manuscript. RFA and TMS helped in systematisation of data, analysed and interpreted the cognitive data and reviewed the manuscript. FLRG followed all the stages of the experiment, discussed the protocols, helped in drafting and reviewed the manuscript. LJMC discussed the data and reviewed the manuscript. DOS discussed the project since its conception, helped in designing the experiments and reviewed the manuscript. CTDR is responsible for conception and design of the study, data analysis and interpretation and helped in drafting and reviewing, and finalised the manuscript together with LPS. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Luciana Pereira de Sousa or Cláudio Tadeu Daniel-Ribeiro.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

The experiments were approved by the Ethics Committee on the Use of Laboratory Animals (CEUA-IOC: L-010/2015) and all procedures related to animal behaviour were performed in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

de Sousa, L., de Almeida, R.F., Ribeiro-Gomes, F.L. et al. Long-term effect of uncomplicated Plasmodium berghei ANKA malaria on memory and anxiety-like behaviour in C57BL/6 mice. Parasites Vectors 11, 191 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13071-018-2778-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13071-018-2778-8

Keywords